题目内容
5.She plays piano every day.piano前面加the.分析 她每天都弹钢琴.
解答 答案:piano前面加the.play作"玩"讲时如果后面跟球类不加the,后面跟乐器时,乐器前面要加定冠词the.
点评 本题考查基础知识点的积累,考试在平时的学习中应注意多读多记,注重一些细节性的基础知识.
![](http://thumb.zyjl.cn/images/loading.gif)
练习册系列答案
相关题目
15.It's a specially protected area.People are not_____ to cut down trees and hunt animals.( )
A. | permited | B. | allowing | C. | advised | D. | permitted |
16.-It is very seldom that one would be invited to a British person's home.
-That's true.Whether the person you know is a work colleague or a neighbor,being invited inside their house is quite ______.( )
-That's true.Whether the person you know is a work colleague or a neighbor,being invited inside their house is quite ______.( )
A. | normal | B. | often | C. | rare | D. | proper |
17.Feeling good about our actions-not guilt or pity-motivates giving,according to the latest research.
(51)Bseeing or hearing about suffering children makes most people uncomfortable,that grief is not what drives them to dig into their pockets and donate.The reasons people decide to be selfless,it turns out,may be slightly more (52)D.
In the study,published in the Journal of Neuroscience,researchers found that people are more likely to give when they think it will make them feel better.They donate,(53)A,when they feel hope about putting smiles on those expectant and suffering faces.And that hope,or similar feel-good sensations,are driven by the brain's reward systems.
Researchers-and charities-have long known that putting a(n) (54)C face on an abstract problem opens hearts and wallets.Josef Stalin once said that while one death is a(n) (55)D,a million is merely a number.Studies have since found that quantifying the size of a disaster or particular need actually (56)C giving,while presenting a single story is more likely to cause a desire to help.
But it wasn't clear whether this"identifiable victim"effect resulted from people's (57)Bover their own privilege and resources-or from a sense of connection with the (58)C and an urge to feel good about making a difference.
To find out,researchers led by Alexander Genevsky,a graduate student in psychology at Stanford,imaged the brains of 22young adults.In the scanner,they saw either a silhouette (剪影) or a head shot of a young African child.As in previous studies,participants were far more likely to give if they saw a face than a blank silhouette-donating almost twice as much in photo trials than in the others.However,this decision was related strongly to their (59)D.If they showed little activity in their nucleus accumbens-a brain region linked to every type of pleasurable experience-they were actually less likely to give.But if there is a sharp (60)Bof activity in this reward area,they felt good and gave more.And the photos of the children were more likely to (61)C this reward center.Activity in the accumbens,in fact,completely (62)C the difference in giving seen between the silhouette-based requests and the photo-based ones.
While the findings point to the feel-good(63)A behind giving,other research will have to address the question of why givers get that positive emotional boost.Do people feel rewarded when they give because they think about the happiness of the recipient-or do they feel good because they see themselves as (64)D and that self-esteem boost (自信心增强) is mood-enhancing?Such information could help charities (65)B their messages to maximize their effectiveness.
(51)Bseeing or hearing about suffering children makes most people uncomfortable,that grief is not what drives them to dig into their pockets and donate.The reasons people decide to be selfless,it turns out,may be slightly more (52)D.
In the study,published in the Journal of Neuroscience,researchers found that people are more likely to give when they think it will make them feel better.They donate,(53)A,when they feel hope about putting smiles on those expectant and suffering faces.And that hope,or similar feel-good sensations,are driven by the brain's reward systems.
Researchers-and charities-have long known that putting a(n) (54)C face on an abstract problem opens hearts and wallets.Josef Stalin once said that while one death is a(n) (55)D,a million is merely a number.Studies have since found that quantifying the size of a disaster or particular need actually (56)C giving,while presenting a single story is more likely to cause a desire to help.
But it wasn't clear whether this"identifiable victim"effect resulted from people's (57)Bover their own privilege and resources-or from a sense of connection with the (58)C and an urge to feel good about making a difference.
To find out,researchers led by Alexander Genevsky,a graduate student in psychology at Stanford,imaged the brains of 22young adults.In the scanner,they saw either a silhouette (剪影) or a head shot of a young African child.As in previous studies,participants were far more likely to give if they saw a face than a blank silhouette-donating almost twice as much in photo trials than in the others.However,this decision was related strongly to their (59)D.If they showed little activity in their nucleus accumbens-a brain region linked to every type of pleasurable experience-they were actually less likely to give.But if there is a sharp (60)Bof activity in this reward area,they felt good and gave more.And the photos of the children were more likely to (61)C this reward center.Activity in the accumbens,in fact,completely (62)C the difference in giving seen between the silhouette-based requests and the photo-based ones.
While the findings point to the feel-good(63)A behind giving,other research will have to address the question of why givers get that positive emotional boost.Do people feel rewarded when they give because they think about the happiness of the recipient-or do they feel good because they see themselves as (64)D and that self-esteem boost (自信心增强) is mood-enhancing?Such information could help charities (65)B their messages to maximize their effectiveness.
51.A.Since | B.Although | C.If | D.As |
52.A.passive | B.earnest | C.impersonal | D.selfish |
53.A.for example | B.on the contrary | C.as a result | D.on the other hand |
54.A.plain | B.ugly | C.specific | D.frightened |
55.A.accident | B.threat | C.solution | D.tragedy |
56.A.ruins | B.stimulates | C.lowers | D.skips |
57.A.anger | B.guilt | C.regret | D.joy |
58.A.desire | B.mind | C.victim | D.stuff |
59.A.actions | B.beliefs | C.images | D.emotions |
60.A.edge | B.rise | C.turn | D.division |
61.A.monitor | B.target | C.activate | D.interrupt |
62.A.resulted from | B.counted on | C.accounted for | D.subjected to |
63.A.motivations | B.compliments | C.ambitions | D.requests |
64.A.executive | B.justified | C.innocent | D.generous |
65.A.conceal | B.tailor | C.obtain | D.delete |
3.When ____ to danger and conflict,men tend to increase blood pressure,______ nervous and anxious.( )
A. | exposed; felt | B. | exposed; feeling | ||
C. | exposing; feeling | D. | exposing; felt |